I am
conducting an on-going online survey regarding Sleep-Related Female Orgasms
and Sex Dreams.� I need responses
from women of all ages, especially ages 60 and above.� If you would like to participate, please
click here.
����������
I have
recently published my new book, Waking into �The Big O� � A New Look at
Sleep-Related Female Orgasms, which includes fascinating statistics and anonymous
commentary from participants in this survey, ages 15 to 85.� It also includes extensive discussion of the conditions
in our present culture which probably contribute to this orgasmic enigma. �I realized that this information needs to be
easily available to the public, and currently there simply is not much
available.� I will continue to gather
data, and plan to eventually update Waking as appropriate.� I also published my doctoral dissertation on Sleep-Related Female Orgasms:
A Survey of Biological, Psychological, Sociological, and Cultural Factors, in
book form.� Each are available at my
e-store for $16.00 plus postage, by clicking on the title, or by ordering
directly from Amazon.com
(SRFOs) or Amazon.com(Waking).�
This material is copyrighted. Please contact me if you would like more
information.
This
page includes the Abstract, Introduction, and modified Table of Contents of my
dissertation for those who might be interested.�
I�ve included the Table of Contents of Waking into �The Big O� on
the survey results page.
Return to my sex therapy and
coaching page
The study summarizes what is known about SRFOs based on existing
research and historical opinion in fields of sexology, physiology, psychology,
sleep, dreaming, anthropology and spirituality.�
While Kinsey noted that there is no single factor or cluster of factors
that is predictive of SRFOs in an individual history, the strongest predictors
in his research were frequent waking orgasm and �erotic responsiveness,� low
availability of other psycho-sexual outlets, masturbation, and fantasy during
masturbation (Kinsey et al. 1953, 212-15).�
Today research suggests that overall, sleep mentations are more
continuous than compensatory, and that sexual content and orgasmic experience
during sleep are more likely among women who think about sex when awake.� Waking cognitions include memory, fantasy,
desire, imagination, prosexual attitudes, knowledge of SRFOs, and
familiarity/safety with sexual pleasure and the orgasmic reflex.� It is likely that formal education,
intelligence, personality characteristics, and other cultural factors also
influence these sleep mentations.�
Orgasmic responses during sleep seem more likely when there is some
level of autonomic nervous system arousal before sleep, including both
psychological and physiological elements.�
Physiological elements include lingering arousal from waking orgasms or
other sexual behavior; however, this arousal may also be due to hormonal
fluctuations, physical exercise, or emotional states such as anxiety, or
anger.� In these latter cases, SRFOs
might serve a compensatory role in maintaining system homeostasis.�� It is likely that SRFOs occur more
frequently among lucid dreamers due to possible neurological conditions unique
to the lucid dream state, and the conscious freedom to exercise volition by
choosing pleasure.� SRFOs appear to be
neither unhealthy nor rare.�
Findings of the study suggest that the term female nocturnal orgasms needs to be replaced with the term� Sleep-Related
Female Orgasms, which is a more accurate designation of the phenomenon
since these occurrences are diurnal as well as nocturnal. Additionally, the
researcher lists twenty-seven possible hypotheses based on an extensive
literature review that could be used in future research on the topic, and
recommends that the topic be included in sex education classes for adolescents
and continuing education modules for heath care professionals.
Erotic dreams among women have been documented since the
early Greek civilization; however, only in the past one hundred years have
researchers begun to study the incidence and dynamics of sleep-related female orgasms
(Kinsey et al. 1953, 191).� While it is
widely known that most men occasionally experience wet dreams, it
appears that many adults in contemporary American culture are unaware that many
women experience sleep-related orgasms.�
Given the prevalence of these responses (37 percent of women by age
forty-five in Kinsey et al. 1953, and likely higher now), this lack of awareness
is somewhat surprising.� Realistically,
however, this topic is rarely discussed.��
In addition, as will be shown, the dynamics and etiology of these
female sleep-related orgasms are somewhat different than the nocturnal
emissions of men.���
Interest in this topic began while testing sample survey
questions for a different topic.� The
issue of female sleep and dream-related orgasms kept surfacing.� Two respondents reported that they mentioned
these occurrences to their male therapists only to be told that the therapists
had �never heard of such a thing.��
Subsequent inquiries have revealed that this is not unusual.� An informal survey by this writer suggests
that in 2005, approximately 70 percent of men did not know that women could
experience sleep-related orgasms.� It is
even more surprising that a significant percentage of women, in excess of 25
percent, lacked this information.
Informal inquiries also suggest that women, who do
experience sleep-related orgasms, enjoy them.�
This writer recently spoke to a mixed-sex group, ages twenties through
sixties, and mentioned that while Kinsey found that the active incidence of
female sleep-related orgasms peaked in the forty and fifty age decades, the
accumulated
incidence
continued to increase throughout the lifespan (Kinsey et al. 1953).� The women in the group started cheering!� Dream therapist Gayle Delaney reports that
she has �never heard a woman tell about an orgasmic dream that was not
pleasurable� (Delaney 1994, 26).� Kinsey
and others have also noted this favorable reaction.
Therefore, one question this paper will address is simply, �Why
don�t more people know that women can, and do, experience sleep-related
orgasms?�� Obviously, lack of education
plays a role.� Contemporary sex education
classes teach boys �such terms as �nocturnal emission� . . . without a parallel
terminology for girls� own nighttime orgasm� (Sweeney 1999).� Although Kinsey found that 5 percent of his
female respondents experienced their first orgasm as a sleep-generated orgasm
(Kinsey et al. 1953, 193), this topic is not included in recommended sex
education curriculums of any organization reviewed by this author for inclusion
at any age level (i.e., American Academy of Pediatrics, 2001, Sexuality
Information and Education Council of the United States, 1996, Kempner [SIECUS]
2003).� Likewise, it is not included in
literature from common public sex education forums such as Planned Parenthood
(www.plannedparenthood.org).� Virtually
all curriculums and information sources include commentary regarding male
nocturnal emissions.� One might conclude
that women typically learn about sleep-related female orgasms through
experience, although this hypothesis has not been tested.� And there still is no accurate, standardized
terminology for this experience in the research or educational literature.�
A common initial response to the above-mentioned question is
that this is another example of how attention to women�s health has been
neglected by the culture and mainstream researchers.� The field of sexology has not been immune
from this charge, as pointed out by Janice Irvine in Disorders of
Desire:� Sex and Gender in Modern
American Sexology:
Scientific sexology responds to feminism by
minimizing it.� The �ignore it and it�ll
go away� approach characterizes an enormous cross-section of American
sexology.� This tactic, perhaps the most
dangerous, is reflected in the virtual absence of feminist analysis and
scholarship within sexual science . . . Structural aspects of sexology
perpetuate male dominance and inhibit feminist intervention.� (Irvine 1990,
144-5)
This topic lies clearly within the scope of sexology; yet, it
interfaces and penetrates other fields of inquiry more deeply than many other
sexological topics.� While research is
therefore more complex, it will ultimately provide greater sexological
insight.� In addition to the normal
physiological, psychological, and cultural variables that relate to sexological
research, the fields of sleep physiology, dreaming, consciousness,
anthropology, and spirituality contribute to awareness and understanding of
this phenomenon.� The mind-body
relationship comes to the forefront providing sharp contrast to the emphasis on
�structural aspects of sexology� mentioned by
Yet, it would be misleading to attribute the lack of awareness
of sleep-related female orgasms to research and educational neglect alone.� This topic has commanded much attention
during other periods of Western civilization.�
Unfortunately, as was the case for sleep, dreams, and sex in general,
the attention was based largely on superstition, fear, and moral judgment.� The most severe consequences came during the
three hundred year witch-burning epoch, mandated by the 1486 publication of the
Catholic Church-initiated document, the Malleus Maleficarum of Kramer
and Sprenger (Stewart 2002, 17-19; Masters 1966).
By the late eighteenth and nineteenth centuries, the sexual
discourse had shifted more fully back into the medical model, its domain during
the Greek and Roman periods, prior to the Christian church (Foucault, 1978,
1985).� While the nature of the moral
judgments and physical punishments changed, sleep-related female orgasms, like
spermatorrhea (male emissions) and masturbation, were nonetheless viewed as
both psychologically and physiologically pathological (Tissot 1758, and others
including Freud 1900) until the 1920s, when they were then thought to be
biologically compensatory for lack of other satisfactory sexual outlets (Kinsey
et al. 1953, 207). To a great extent, Kinsey�s data dispelled the compensatory
imperative as a primary causative factor, thus leaving confusion, mystery, and
relative silence.� The dominant theory
since Kinsey�s time could be called the system maintenance model
(Reinisch 1990, 89), which implies a randomness not really supported by the
meager information at hand.
�As Charles Stewart of the
Royal Anthropological Institute points out:
Erotic dreams have raised perennial questions
about the boundaries of the self and individual�s ability to control and
produce this self.� Do erotic dreams
result from divine intercession, an immoral life, or recent memories?� Are they products of the self for which the
individual dreamer may be held responsible?�
Or are they determined by a force majeure such as original sin, or human
physiology?� (Stewart 2002, 2)
This paper provides literature review research regarding
Sleep-Related Female Orgasms (SRFOs) and factors associated with these
responses, research regarding the dynamics of female sex dreams (which often
precede SRFOs), the history of attitudes and cultural responses toward SRFOs,
and possible reasons why knowledge of SRFOs is apparently withheld from both
men and women. It will explore social, behavioral, physiological,
psychological, spiritual, moral, and political factors that influence and
relate to this response, and make recommendations regarding further research
and educational initiatives.
INTRODUCTION...1
CHAPTER 1:� DEFINITIONS AND EXAMPLES...6
CHAPTER 2: A
REVIEW OF CONTEMPORARY SURVEY RESEARCH...15
CHAPTER 3: THE
PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP-RELATED FEMALE ORGASMS...28
CHAPTER 4:
BEHAVIORAL FACTORS RELATED TO SRFOs...54
CHAPTER 5: THE
ROLE OF DREAMS IN SRFOs...68
CHAPTER 5:
OTHER PSYCHOLOGICAL AND BEHAVIORAL FACTORS RELATED TO SRFOs...90
CHAPTER 7:
SPIRITUAL, RELIGIOUS, SOCIAL, AND POLITICAL CONSIDERATIONS REGARDING
SRFOs...114
CHAPTER 8: SUMMARY
AND RECOMMENDATIONS...145
Dream Incubation �
by Gillian Holloway, PhD...161
REFERENCES...163